Curriculum Vitaes

Souta Hidaka

  (日髙 聡太)

Profile Information

Affiliation
Faculty of Human Sciences Department of Psychology, Sophia University
Degree
Doctor (Philosophy)(Tohoku University)

Researcher number
40581161
ORCID ID
 https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6727-5322
J-GLOBAL ID
201101066235007859
researchmap Member ID
B000000665

External link

Committee Memberships

 7

Papers

 57
  • Kyuto Uno, Souta Hidaka
    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, Jan 3, 2024  Peer-reviewed
  • 日高聡太, 川越 敏和, 浅井 暢子, 寺本 渉
    心理学研究, Dec, 2023  Peer-reviewedLead authorCorresponding author
  • Souta Hidaka, Miyu Takeshima, Toshikazu Kawagoe
    i-Perception, 14(6) 1-14, Nov, 2023  Peer-reviewedLead authorCorresponding author
  • Souta Hidaka, Na Chen, Naomi Ishii, Risa Iketani, Kirino Suzuki, Matthew R. Longo, Makoto Wada
    Autism Research, 16(9) 1750-1764, Jul 6, 2023  Peer-reviewedLead authorCorresponding author
    Abstract People with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or higher levels of autistic traits have atypical characteristics in sensory processing. Atypicalities have been reported for proprioceptive judgments, which are tightly related to internal bodily representations underlying position sense. However, no research has directly investigated whether self‐bodily representations are different in individuals with ASD. Implicit hand maps, estimated based on participants' proprioceptive sensations without sight of their hand, are known to be distorted such that the shape is stretched along the medio‐lateral hand axis even for neurotypical participants. Here, with the view of ASD as falling on a continuous distribution among the general population, we explored differences in implicit body representations along with autistic traits by focusing on relationships between autistic traits and the magnitudes of the distortions in implicit hand maps (N ~ 100). We estimated the magnitudes of distortions in implicit hand maps both for fingers and hand surfaces on the dorsal and palmar sides of the hand. Autistic traits were measured by questionnaires (Autism Spectrum [AQ] and Empathy/Systemizing [EQ‐SQ] Quotients). The distortions in implicit hand maps were replicated in our experimental situations. However, there were no significant relationships between autistic traits and the magnitudes of the distortions as well as within‐individual variabilities in the maps and localization performances. Consistent results were observed from comparisons between IQ‐matched samples of people with and without a diagnosis of ASD. Our findings suggest that there exist perceptual and neural processes for implicit body representations underlying position sense consistent across levels of autistic traits.
  • Souta Hidaka, Mizuho Gotoh, Shinya Yamamoto, Makoto Wada
    Scientific Reports, 13(1), Apr 11, 2023  Peer-reviewedLead authorCorresponding author
    Abstract The number of clinical diagnoses of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is increasing annually. Interestingly, the human body temperature has also been reported to gradually decrease over the decades. An imbalance in the activation of the excitatory and inhibitory neurons is assumed to be involved in the pathogenesis of ASD. Neurophysiological evidence showed that brain activity decreases as cortical temperature increases, suggesting that an increase in brain temperature enhances the inhibitory neural mechanisms. Behavioral characteristics specific to clinical ASD were observed to be moderated when people with the diagnoses had a fever. To explore the possible relationship between ASD and body temperature in the general population, we conducted a survey study using a large population-based sample (N ~ 2000, in the age groups 20s to 70s). Through two surveys, multiple regression analyses did not show significant relationships between axillary temperatures and autistic traits measured by questionnaires (Autism Spectrum (AQ) and Empathy/Systemizing Quotients), controlling for covariates of age and self-reported circadian rhythms. Conversely, we consistently observed a negative relationship between AQ and age. People with higher AQ scores tended to have stronger eveningness. Our findings contribute to the understanding of age-related malleability and the irregularity of circadian rhythms related to autistic traits.

Misc.

 29
  • Hidaka Souta, Saegusa Chihiro
    The Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science, 41(2) 144-145, Mar 31, 2023  
    This note is a short report on the activities of the Young Researchers Committee of the Japanese Psychonomic Society (JPS). As the highlighted activity of the committee, we have held the oral session of the annual meeting of JPS both face-to-face and online formats nine times. We also have promoted interactive activities across the country through social networking services. By introducing public applications, we have tried to organize our committee with diverse members. We will continue to develop new activities to target a broader range of young or early-career researchers.
  • 日髙 聡太, 浅野 倫子
    日本心理学会大会発表論文集, 86 ITL-001-ITL-001, 2022  
    知覚・認知処理の目的は,外界から入力された情報を我々にとって有用な形に変換,表現することにあると考えられる。知覚・認知処理において複数の感覚情報を組み合わせて利用することで,信頼性のある頑健な表現を構築することが可能となる。本講演では,講演者がこれまで行ってきた多感覚を対象とした実験心理学的研究の成果を概観する。ある感覚が別の感覚に影響を及ぼすという相互作用に加えて,複数の感覚にまたがって類似した処理特性を持つという共通性についても触れながら,人の知覚・認知処理の動作原理について考察する。
  • Souta Hidaka, Masakazu Ide
    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 51 69-70, Jul, 2016  
  • IKEDA Hanako, TANAKA Chiaki, HIDAKA Souta, ISHIYAMA Tomohiro, MIYAZAKI Genta
    Technical report of IEICE. HIP, 114(347) 1-6, Dec 1, 2014  
    The present study examined how differences in resolution (4K and HD imaging) could influence subjective impressions of movies in association with movie contents (natural/artificial objects) and fields of view (wide/medium/narrow). We found that 4K movies induced stronger impressions regarding evaluation and comfort especially when they were presented with natural scene and/or larger field of view. These results suggest that the image resolutions could modulate subjective impressions of movies in accordance with the differences in movie contents and fields of view.
  • W. Teramoto, S. Takebe, S. Hidaka
    PERCEPTION, 43(1) 36-36, Aug, 2014  
  • TERAMOTO Wataru, YOSHIDA Kazuhiro, ASAI Nobuko, HIDAKA Souta, GYOBA Jiro, SAKAMOTO Shuichi, IWAYA Yukio, SUZUKI Yoiti
    ITE Technical Report, 36(12) 37-44, Feb 27, 2012  
    Recent advancement of information and communication technologies has evoked expectations for more natural and realistic communications. To this end, the enhancement of a sense of presence-subjective experience of being in one place even when one is physically situated in another-has been the most important issue. In the present study we first discuss the essence of sense of presence based on our recent survey of more than 200 non-researchers. Next, we show spatio-temporal characteristics for vraisemblance, which is the reality or virtuality assumed to link essentially to foreground components in a scene, while contrasting it with the sense of presence, which, theoretically and empirically, has been found to relate dominantly to background components contained in a scene. Lastly we introduce most recent research on a sense of being together, which is assumed to be one of the essential issues in communication with other persons in a virtual environment.
  • SUZUKI Yoiti, TERAMOTO Wataru, YOSHIDA Kazuhiro, ASAI Nobuko, HIDAKA Souta, SAKAMOTO Shuichi, IWAYA Yukio, GYOBA Jiro
    IEICE technical report, 111(74) 29-36, May 23, 2011  
    Recent advancement of information and communication technologies has evoked expectations for more natural and realistic communications. In studies on multimedia/multimodal communications, the enhancement of a sense of presence-a subjective experience of being in one place even when one is physically situated in another-has been the most important issue. Physical factors for high presence and objective measures of sense of presence have been intensively investigated over twenty years, but it is less known yet. In the present study we first discuss the essence of sense of presence based on our recent survey of more than 200 non-researchers. Next, we show spatio-temporal characteristics for vraisemblance, which is the reality or virtuality assumed to link essentially to foreground components in a scene, while contrasting it with the sense of presence, which, theoretically and empirically, has been found to relate dominantly to background components contained in a scene. Lastly we discuss future directions of measurements for higher order states of sensation such as sense of presence and vraisemblance, and its possible contributions to human-machine interfaces, communications, and entertainment technologies.
  • W. Teramoto, M. Kobayashi, S. Hidaka, Y. Sugita
    PERCEPTION, 40 186-186, 2011  
  • W. Teramoto, S. Hidaka, Y. Sugita, S. Sakamoto, J. Gyoba, Y. Iwaya, Y. Suzuki
    PERCEPTION, 39 143-143, 2010  
  • YOSHIDA Kazuhiro, TERAMOTO Wataru, ASAI Nobuko, HIDAKA Souta, SAKAMOTO Shuichi, GYOBA Jiro, SUZUKI Yoiti
    Technical report of IEICE. HIP, 109(345) 91-96, Dec 10, 2009  
    We investigated shared understandings of sense of presence among students in science and engineering courses. The studens tended to use the term, "sense of presence", not only to refer to an experience of being there in a mediated environment, but also refer to an experience which makes them excited or strained in an actual environment. The students estimated that the visual, auditory, vestibular and kinesthetic senses were closely related to sense of presence. Furthermore, a semantic differential method revealed four different elements of sense of presence-evaluation, impact/activity, stimulus structure and extraordinariness. The first two results (the diversity of the meaning and the sensory modality selectivity of sense of presence) were consistent with those in our previous study which had investigated the understandings of sense of presence among students in arts and humanities courses. However, the factor structure of sense of presence was different between these two studies. This result suggests that educational backgrounds may affect the understandings of sense of presence.
  • TAKAHASHI Junichi, HIDAKA Souta, TERAMOTO Wataru, GYOBA Jiro
    IEICE technical report, 109(345) 109-114, Dec 10, 2009  
    Repetition Blindness (RB) is the phenomenon in which observers fail to report the repeated second target (T2) which is identical to the first target (T1) in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP). It has been assumed that the encoding of the T1 representation interferes with that of the T2 representation so that RB for T2 occurs. Furthermore, it was also reported that RB was reduced when T1 and T2 was accompanied with transient auditory stimuli. The aim of this study was to investigate whether the redundancy of visual pattern modulates the reduction of RB induced by sounds. The results showed that the reduction of the RB was observed only when the visual stimuli contained lower redundancy. Furthermore, the reduction of the RB was more salient when the sound was presented with T1 rather than T2. These results suggest that the audiovisual interaction on visual RB changes with the redundancy contained in the visual patterns.
  • W. Teramoto, S. Hidaka, J. Gyoba, Y-I Suzuki
    PERCEPTION, 38 132-132, 2009  
  • YOSHIDA Kazuhiro, TERAMOTO Wataru, ASAI Nobuko, HIDAKA Souta, GYOBA Jiro, SUZUKI Yoiti
    IEICE technical report, 108(356) 53-58, Dec 11, 2008  
    We investigated shared understandings of "sense of presence" among non-researchers by a questionnaire method. Although more than 90% of them recognized the term, "sense of presence", the term was used not only to refer to an experience of "being there" in a mediated environment, but also refer to an actual experience which shakes their hearts. An SD (semantic differential) method revealed four different elements of "sense of presence" -evaluation, impact, activity and mechanicalness. Events with high presence were likely to be evaluated as preferable, impressive, and dynamic. Furthermore, "sense of presence" was estimated to have a close relation to the visual, auditory, vestibular and kinesthetic senses. These results suggest that these aspects of "sense of presence" should be taken into account in specifying physical factors for high-presence and establishing objective measures for sense of presence.
  • HIDAKA Souta, TERAMOTO Wataru, GYOBA Jiro, SUZUKI Yoiti
    IEICE technical report, 108(356) 111-116, Dec 11, 2008  
    In visual apparent motion display, an abrupt change in object attributes such as size extends the visible time of the changed object even after its physical termination (visible persistence). The aim of this study was to investigate whether and how an abrupt change in an attribute of contingent auditory stimuli can also alter the visible persistence of a visual object. The visible persistence occurred for longer time when an abrupt change of tone was introduced into a sequence of constant tones presented synchronously with visual stimuli than when no, abrupt change or no auditory information was introduced (Experiment 1). However, an abrupt onset of a tone burst or an abrupt offset in a sequence of tone bursts did not affect the visible persistence. This confirmed that the results of Experiment 1 did not base on the perceived intensity changes of the visual stimuli, attentional capture to the visual changes, or response biases induced by transient auditory signal (Experiments 2 and 3). Furthermore, the effect of an abrupt change in tone on the visible persistence was attenuated when the auditory information was difficult to be attributed to the moving visual objects (Experiment 4). These results suggest that the object representations underlying the visible persistence might be formed by the multisensory integration of auditory and visual information.
  • MARUYAMA Suguru, KONNO Akitsugu, HIDAKA Souta, SHIBATA Hiroshi, KURIHARA Michiyo, TANAKA Akihiro, KOIZUMI Masatoshi, GYOBA Jiro, HAGIWARA Hiroko
    IEICE technical report, 108(184) 41-44, Aug 1, 2008  
    We measured the preschoolers' brain activities during listening to a story in Japanese (the first language), English (the second language), and Chinese (the unexposed foreign language) by using near-infrared spectroscopy. The preschoolers of 3-4 and 4-5 years old with lower exposure to English showed no significant brain activity regardless of the language they heard. In contrast, the preschoolers of 4-5 and 5-6 years old with higher exposure to English revealed significantly more brain activity for Japanese and English stories than for Chinese. These results suggest that the higher exposure to English facilitates the brain activity responsible for second language processing.
  • KONNO Akitsugu, MARUYAMA Suguru, HIDAKA Souta, TANAKA Akihiro, KOIZUMI Masatoshi, GYOBA Jiro, HAGIWARA Hiroko
    IEICE technical report, 108(184) 45-48, Aug 1, 2008  
    We observed the preschoolers' brain activities in listening to /ra/ and /la/ phonemic contrast by using near-infrared spectroscopy. Based on an auditory oddball paradigm, we used two types of auditory stimulus period where /ra/ phonemes were repeated, and where /ra/ and /la/ phonemes were randomly presented. In the participants of 3-4 years old, there was no difference in brain activities regardless of their English experiences. In the participants of 4-5 years old, on the other hand, the preschoolers with higher exposure to English showed more significant brain activities in their left hemisphere than those with lower exposure to English. These results suggest that English experiences affect the developmental changes of brain activities for phonemic processing of the second language.
  • W. Teramoto, S. Hidaka, J. Gyoba, Y. Suzuki
    PERCEPTION, 37 72-72, 2008  
  • HONDA Akio, SHIBATA Hiroshi, HIDAKA Souta, GYOBA Jiro, IWAYA Yukio, SUZUKI Yoiti
    IEICE technical report, 107(369) 49-54, Nov 29, 2007  
    We investigated the effects of listener's head movement and kinetic feedback on the accuracy of sound localization. The effects were examined under the conditions that their head movement was restricted (study 1) or unrestricted (study 2) on the sound localization training. In both experiments, participants divided into two groups : training group performed the sound localization training with the kinetic feedback, while control group conducted the training with no-feedback. The results indicated that : (1) sound localization learning under active head movements increased the accuracy of sound localization ; (2) kinetic feedback facilitated the sound localization during the initial learning phase ; and (3) training with kinetic feedback dominantly decreased the rate of vertical localization errors in sound localization performance.
  • 日高聡太, 河地庸介, 行場次朗
    北海道心理学研究, (29) 106, Aug 31, 2007  
  • 本多明生, 柴田寛, 日高聡太, 行場次朗, 岩谷幸雄, 鈴木陽一
    日本バーチャルリアリティ学会大会論文集(CD-ROM), 12th, 2007  
  • S. Hidaka, Y. Kawachi, J. Gyoba
    PERCEPTION, 36 100-101, 2007  
  • Hidaka Souta, Kawachi Yousuke, Gyoba Jiro
    Proceedings of the Japanese Society for Cognitive Psychology, 2007 111-111, 2007  
    The current research investigated depth information contained in the representation of apparently moving 3-D objects. We measured the magnitude of representational momentum (RM) as index of the consistency of object's shape. Experiment 1A found that RM magnitude was higher when shaded convex moving objects shifted to a flat circle than when that shifted to a shaded concave hemisphere. This difference diminished in the case that the moving objects were the concave (Experiment 1B). Experiment 2 confirmed that these results did not stem from luminance information of shading using luminance-polarized circles. Moreover, Experiment 3 showed that RM magnitude was higher when the convex moving objects shifted to blurred convex hemispheres with low-pass filters than when that shifted to the concave hemispheres. The results suggest that internal representations contain incomplete depth information intermediate between those of 2-D and 3-D objects especially for convexity with low-spatial frequency dominance properties in apparent motion.
  • 日高 聡太, 河地 庸介, 行場 次朗
    The Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science, 24(2) 238-238, 2006  
  • 日高 聡太, 河地 庸介, 行場 次朗
    基礎心理学研究, 25(1) 129-129, 2006  
  • 日高聡太, 河地庸介, 行場次朗
    電子情報通信学会技術研究報告, 105(479(HIP2005 79-106)) 115-120, Dec 8, 2005  

Books and Other Publications

 6
  • 日本視覚学会 (Role: Contributor, 第V章「多感覚認知」・第5.2節「視覚から聴覚への作用」)
    朝倉書店, Nov, 2022 (ISBN: 9784254102949)
  • 日高聡太, 北川智利 (Role: Contributor, 第11章「感覚間相互作用」)
    コロナ社, Apr, 2021 (ISBN: 9784339013658)
  • Teramoto, W, Hidaka, S, Sugita, Y (Role: Contributor, Auditory bias in visual motion perception)
    Cambridge University Press, 2018
  • 日髙 聡太 (Role: Contributor, 第1章「原理・歴史第3節「19世紀後半における近代心理学の成立」)
    誠信書房, 2014
  • 日髙 聡太 (Role: Contributor, 第2章「感覚・知覚心理学」・第6節「神経生理学的理論」.)
    朝倉書店, 2012

Presentations

 17

Teaching Experience

 20

Research Projects

 14